Skip to main content

Relationship between body image, anxiety, food-specific inhibitory control, and emotional eating in young women with abdominal obesity: a comparative cross-sectional study

Abstract

Background

Abdominal fat deposition is a key component of obesity, which is associated with an increased risk for a number of mental disorders. The current study aims to explore the relationship between body image, anxiety, food-specific inhibitory control, and emotional eating in young women with abdominal obesity.

Method

A total of 224 participants were recruited: 168 were non-abdominal obesity and 56 were abdominal obesity. Participants completed the following questionnaires and behavioral tests: the Body Mass Index (BMI) -based Silhouette-Matching Test (SMT), the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI), Food Stop Signal Task (SST), the Emotional Eating Scale (EES).

Results

Abdominal obesity women had significantly higher levels of trait anxiety, cognitive difference, expectational difference in body image but lower self-reported emotional eating level compared to the control group. Anxiety mediated the relationship between cognitive difference of body image and depression eating in young females with abdominal obesity. In addition, only among abdominal obesity individuals, expectational difference of body image were significantly and positively correlated with food-specific inhibitory control and trait/state anxiety.

Conclusion

The findings suggest it is of critical importance to promote a healthy body image recognition and expectation and improve mood regulation for young females with abdominal obesity high in trait anxiety.

Peer Review reports

Background

Abdominal fat deposition is a key component of obesity [1]. Some studies have shown that abdomen circumference (AC) may be a better predictor for the risk of type 2 diabetes, medical care costs, and all-cause mortality than body mass index (BMI) [2,3,4,5]. Abdominal obesity are characterized with excessive body fat in abdomen circumference [6]. The prevalence of visceral fat that accumulates around abdominal organs is increasing worldwide [7, 8]. Visceral fat cells have an crucial impact on overall health and well-being. Inappropriate diets such as high-calorie foods are other main influential risk factors for increasing abdominal obesity. Consequently, AC has emerged as a candidate for assessing abdominal obesity. In fact, AC has been reported to predict mortality risk better than BMI [9]. The AC is positively correlated with the abdominal fat. Hence, the AC is a valuable, convenient and a simple measurement method which can be used for identifying the individuals who are at an increased risk for the above mentioned diseases. The identification of the abdominal obesity by the abdomen circumference measurement is easily accessible and should become the obligatory part of any physical examination [10]. AC, frequently used as a simple, inexpensive measure of central obesity in population-based studies, has been shown to be associated with depression in some studies [11].

Eating behaviors, negative emotions (i.e., anxiety and angry), inhibitory control, and body image perception are implicated in the multifactorial psychological factors of obesity [12,13,14,15,16]. Obesity has been associated with an increased lifetime risk for major depression and panic disorder or agoraphobia, particularly among females [17]. They always use food to cope with stress and emotions. Eating has been recognized as a coping mechanism for alleviating and dealing with stress and emotions [18] by either undereating or overeating [19].

Body image has been demonstrated to be associated with obesity either as a cause or as a result that impacts on weight control behaviors [20, 21]. Perceptual body size misperception (either underestimation or overestimation) occurs more in individuals with greater obesity, and it can potentially lead to a lesser awareness of the health risks associated with obesity and a reduction in the implementation of weight control behaviors such as dieting [22].

Altered inhibitory control has been implicated in obesity. Several studies concluded that obesity and binge-related eating disorders (EDs) are associated with poor inhibitory control [23, 24]. The stop signal task (SST) was chosen as it is commonly used to assess motor control, with cognitive underpinnings that are clearly established and may have relevance to eating and weight-control behaviors (e.g., cognitive control exercised when resisting urges to eat) [25]. However, in both adults and youth, the findings were largely inconsistent. Three studies reported greater SSRTs (Stop Signal Reaction Time) in obese adults and overweight individuals [26, 27]. In contrast, some studies found no overall differences in SSRT between normal weight groups and overweight/ obese adults [26, 28,29,30,31].

Emotional eating has been observed in both obese individuals [32] and a critical review of the literature concluded that there is no relationship between body mass index and emotional eating [33]. Thus, vulnerability to emotional eating does not appear to be simply a function of weight. Emotional eating is likely effected by other psychological factors. Dysfunctional eating behaviors appeared to correlate strongly with body dissatisfaction, and perfectionism in girls [34]. In addition to body image, individual differences in affective traits and states may account for some of the observed variability in the effects of emotions on eating [35].

Several studies have found that obesity were more concerned about their physical appearance (body dissatisfaction and obsession with being thin) [36]. In fact, some authors argue that these types of cognitive variables could explain the vulnerability shown in people who go on to develop an eating disorder [37]. Likewise, common mental disorders were associated with an increased risk of obesity, and that the risk of obesity increased with the number of episodes of anxiety [38]. To be more specific, several studies have shown that youths who feel bad physically also feel bad emotionally [39]. Jansen et al. [40] reported that obese individuals high in negative affect consumed more food-specific than individuals low in negative affect following a negative mood induction, relative to a neutral mood induction. In contrast, lean individuals consumed comparable amounts of calories in the negative and neutral mood induction conditions, regardless of their level of negative affect [40]. With regard to this question, Braet et al. [41] point out that different psychological mechanisms and patterns seem to be in place in obesity female versus the control group. Considering the conclusion that high trait anxiety was positively associated with food-specific intake for obese individuals, but not their lean counterparts [35], it was possible to infer that the obesity group is more vulnerable to developing eating disorders, as those obesity female generally show more body image dissatisfaction, worse food-specific inhibition, and higher levels of anxiety regarding their body and weight and follow unhealthy diet administration. Therefore, anxiety is inferred to be the psychological mechanisms and patterns of of inappropriate emotional eating in obese youths [42].

More than half of the females preferred their ideal figure to be underweight, whereas about 30% males chose an overweight figure as their ideal model. Females were generally more concerned about body weight, body shape and eating than males. As suggested in the aforementioned studies, obesity female may be significant risk conditions, especially in youth, associated with inappropriate weight-control behaviors, emotional distress (anxiety, depressive symptoms, etc.) and concerns about one’s own body image (body dissatisfaction, negative beliefs about one’s body and eating, etc.) [43]. In addition, in China, despite having high obesity prevalence rates and more serious abdominal obesity prevalence rates [44,45,46], no studies have been carried out to analyse body image dissatisfaction, inappropriate emotional eating behaviors, food-specific inhibitory control and their relationship with variables of emotional distress according to whether they are abdominal obesity. In addition to state anxiety, trait anxiety may also be a risk factor for emotional eating among obese individuals [35].

Although the prevalence of overall obesity as measured by BMI is well-documented and it has increased dramatically in the past 2 decades [47, 48], little is known about the psychological characteristics of abdominal obesity in young women. Taking these into account, The present study established the following objectives: (a) to compare the level of inhibitory control, emotional eating, anxiety and body image between abdominal obesity and non-abdominal obesity in young women, (b) to compare abdomen circumference -related differences in relation between the level of inhibitory control, emotional eating, anxiety and body image in abdominal obesity and non-abdominal obesity groups, (c) to test the hypothesis that anxiety is the psychological mechanisms and patterns of of inappropriate emotional eating in obese youths.

Methods and materials

Study design

A comparative crossectional study approach was used.

Participants

Xi’an was the place where this study was conducted. It is an important central city in western of China. The study was run beginning from May 2th, to October 20nd, 2019. All the participants were recruited through advertisements at local universities. They were selected by convenient sampling to participate in anonymous questionnaires survey and behavioral experimental tasks. The inclusion criteria were; being aged 18–25. In this study, the term ‘young women’ refers to the study population at hand with individuals aged 18–25. Young women were excluded from the study when the target individuals had a serious handicap, visual impairment, chronic neurological disorder (e.g., mental retardation), or psychiatric disorder.

The sample consisted of 224 young women who signed an informed consent. Of the 224 participants, 25% were abdominal obesity individuals (Non-abdominal Obesity: 168, abdomen circumference ≤ 85 cm; Abdominal Obesity: 56, abdomen circumference ≥ 85 cm) [49]. They studied in college in Xi’an city and received small presents for their participation. None reported significant medical impairments or physical and mental illness.

Procedure

These female youth accepted tests in a quiet room individually. They completed tasks in a fixed order: demographic, height, weight and body image perceptions (the body mass index -based Silhouette-Matching Test), anxiety (state-trait anxiety inventory), Food- inhibitory control (the Food- Stop Signal Task), emotional eating (the Emotional Eating Scale). Each measure is described in detail in the following section.

Measures

Body image perceptions

Body image attitude was measured by the body mass index (BMI)-based Silhouette-Matching Test (SMT; [50]). In this test, participants were presented with silhouettes of figures ranging from very slim to very full, and were required to choose a number below the figures (1 to 27) to indicate (a) their current figure, (b) the ideal figure which they would like to have. In the current study, participants’ selection of the current figure was significantly correlated with current BMI, as calculated from a participant’s reported height and weight (r = .74, p < .001, df = 224). The degree of body image dissatisfaction was quantified with two measures: (1) Cognitive difference of body image (Cognitive 27-point SMT-BMI): the absolute value of this difference as some individuals may prefer a fuller image of themselves; and (2) Expectational difference of body image (Cognitive 27-point SMT-Expectational 27-point SMT): the difference between current and ideal body image (subjective drive for thinness). Similar methods have been used previously [51].

Anxiety

Levels of anxiety were measured with the Chinese version of the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI) S-Anxiety Scale [52, 53]. There are 2 subscales within this measure. The State Anxiety Scale (S-Anxiety) evaluates the current state of anxiety, asking how respondents feel “right now,” using items that measure subjective feelings of apprehension, tension, nervousness, worry, and activation/ arousal of the autonomic nervous system. The Trait Anxiety Scale (T-Anxiety) evaluates relatively stable aspects of “anxiety proneness,” including general states of calmness, confidence, and security. The STAI has 40 items, 20 items allocated to each of the S-Anxiety and T-Anxiety subscales. All items are self-rated on a 4-point scale of 1 = not at all, 2 = somewhat, 3 = moderately and 4 = very much, and they are added to obtain a total score for each respondent. Total scores range from 20 to 80, with higher scores indicating higher levels of anxiety. The scale has been tested and validated for the Chinese context. The Cronbach’s alpha level was acceptable (.82 and 0.79).

Food- inhibitory control

In the Food-stop signal task [27, 54], participants need to classify continuously presented stimuli according to simple criteria by key press. A stop signal is presented unpredictably in a random subset of trials following the display of the imperative stimulus and before the anticipated response. Participants are instructed not to execute the response in these trials. The paradigm is based on the theoretical horse-race model 55], which assumes independent go- and stop-processes. If the stop-process terminates before the go-process, a response is effectively inhibited. Good stopping performance is reflected in a fast stop-process which can be initiated late and which will, therefore, still terminate an ongoing response tendency. Participants were instructed to select as quickly and accurately as possible on which side the picture appeared by pressing either a left or a right response button. In the low calorie and high calorie food- stop signal task, there were respectively 3 kinds of photographs of shapes (e.g., quadrate, roundness, etc.), low-calorie food items (e.g., tomato, apples, etc.) and high calorie food items (e.g., fried chickens, desserts, etc.). A picture appeared in either on the left or right side of the fixation cross. A left or a right response button respectively represents a response to specific stimulus. In stop trials, a small blueberry /doughnut appeared over the picture as the stop signal after a variable delay. Upon any response detected or after 1500 ms, the screen was cleared. The recorded scores reflected “stop” accuracy in stop trials. The dependent variable, stop signal reaction time (SSRT), was calculated by subtracting the mean stop delay from mean reaction times. Higher SSRTs indicate decreased inhibitory control. Higher stop signal accuracy (SSACC) indicated better performance.

Emotional eating

The emotional eating scale [56] has been used to investigate eating behavior in response to negative emotions. We used the Chinese version of the Emotional Eating Scale [57], which contains four factors: eating in response to anxiety (four items), depression (nine items), anger/hostility (five items) and positive emotion (five items). Participants were asked to respond to questions about their desire to eat when experiencing certain emotions (e.g., “Do you have a desire to eat when you are sad, irritated, worried, or lonely?”) on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (never) to 5 (very strong). Higher scores indicate a stronger desire to eat. In the current study, Cronbach’s α was 0.73(anxiety) -0.80 (anger/hostility) for each emotional eating scale.

Covariates

We adjusted for age, BMI, family socioeconomic status (SES). Although ideally, all participants should have provided information including all three components (education, occupation, and income) of SES, the majority of students, especially in China, only know and are more willing to provide information about education and occupation than income data [58]. Therefore, our study only considered the education and occupational prestige of participants’ parents. These were assigned based on categories of the Hollingshead Index matched as closely as possible to modern education and occupation status [59]. The family SES composite index was the sum value of parents’ SES.

Statistical analysis

The descriptive statistics, correlations and path analysis were computed using SPSS 16.0 and AMOS 7.0 software [60]. Depending on the scale of the variable, the mean, standard deviations, and proportions were presented as a descriptive summary. And a comparison of the abdominal obesity and non-abdominal obesity groups was conducted through independent-samples t test. Two separate correlation analysis were run. Partial correlations was used to assess whether there are association differences of inhibitory control, emotional eating, anxiety and body image between abdominal obesity and non-abdominal obesity in young women.

Mediation tests indicated whether the association between two variables resulted from another variable or a set of variables. Path analysis is a specific tool of the structural equation model (SEM) analysis to analyze assumed relationships of multivariate data. Mediation tests were computed using path analysis in AMOS 7.0 with maximum likelihood estimation to examine the significance of the direct effects of cognitive difference of body image on depression eating in the female abdominal obesity group, mediated through the anxiety. The structural equation model as illustrated (see Fig. 1) was tested for indirect effects of anxiety with a bias-corrected bootstrapping procedure based on 2000 bootstrap samples to estimate standardized regression estimates. For a good fit, the degrees of freedom (χ2/df) should be as small as possible, and values less than three indicate a good or acceptable fit. Goodness of fit indices were assessed based on following criteria: Tucker Lewis index (TLI) and comparative fit index (CFI) close to 0.9 and Root Mean Square Error of Approximation (RMSEA) < 0.8 [61].

Fig. 1
figure 1

Trait anxiety mediation effects in the relationships between cognitive difference of body image and depression eating in the female abdominal obesity group controlling for age, BMI, and family SES. Significant paths from cognitive difference of body image to depression eating (using bias-corrected bootstrapped confidence intervals). Mean path coefficients were obtained using 2000 bootstrap samples. Note. *p < .05. **p < .01.***p < .001

Results

Body image, anxiety, food- inhibitory control and emotional eating

Cognitive difference and expectational difference of body image were greater than zero. All the participants (100%) perceived themselves to be fatter than physical truth and wanted a more slender figure. Participants of both groups were equivalent in age. However, no significant difference between the groups with respect to the stop signal accuracy (shape, low calorie, high calorie food), the stop signal reaction time (shape, low calorie, high calorie food), depression eating and anger/hostility eating. Abdominal obesity female youths reported higher levels of cognitive difference and expectational difference of body image (t = − 2.05, p < .01; t = − 11.19, p < .001), while non-abdominal obesity ones reported higher emotional eating scores about anxiety eating and positive emotion eating (t = 3.46, p < .01; t = 4.34, p < .01) (Table 1).

Table 1 Body Image, Anxiety, Food- inhibitory Control and Emotional Eating of Two Abdomen Circumference Groups

Partial correlations between body image, anxiety and the stop signal task

Partial correlation analysis showed that for abdominal obesity female youths (abdomen circumference ≥ 85 cm), the correlation between expectational difference of body image, trait anxiety, state anxiety, SSACC Low Calorie, and SSACC High Calorie were significant (r = .59, p < .001; r = .47, p < .001; r = .32, p < .05; r = .40, p < .01;). For non-abdominal obesity ones (abdomen circumference ≤ 85 cm), the correlation between cognitive difference of body image, SSACC Low Calorie, and SSACC High Calorie were significant (r = .18, p < .05; r = .23, p < .01). However, the correlation between expectational difference of body image, trait anxiety, SSACC Low Calorie, and SSACC High Calorie were not significant. For non-abdominal obesity ones, the correlation between body image and most emotional eating scores were negatively correlated. On the contrary, in the abdominal obesity group, the correlation between cognitive difference of body image, expectational difference of body image and depression eating were positively correlated (r = .41, p < .01; r = .28, p < .05) (Table 2).

Table 2 Partial correlations between body image, anxiety, the stop signal task (SST) and emotional eating controlling for age, BMI, and family SES

Partial correlations between anxiety and emotional eating

As presented in Table 3, overall trait anxiety and state anxiety was correlated with depression eating and anger/hostility eating. Analyzing by non-abdominal obesity & abdominal obesity separately, partial correlations between trait anxiety, state anxiety and depression eating were significant (r = .58, p < .001; r = 51, p < .001) for abdominal obesity female undergraduates. And partial correlations between trait anxiety and anxiety eating were also significant (r = .33, p < .05). In a parallel analysis of the non-abdominal obesity group, only the correlation between trait anxiety and anger/hostility eating was significant (r = .19, p < .05).

Table 3 Partial correlations between anxiety and emotional eating controlling for age, BMI, and family SES

Mediation results

The structural equation model provided a good fit to the sample data with TLI = .98, CFI = 0.94, RMSEA = .00,χ2/df = .88. Cognitive difference of body image predicted the mediating variables trait anxiety and state anxiety (β = .38, 95% CI: 0.10–0.62, p < .01; β = .50, 95% CI: 0.21–0.74, p < .01). There was significant direct influence of cognitive difference of body image (β = .38, 95% CI: 0.05–0.71, p < .05), as the predicted variable, on depression eating. As predictive indirect effect of mediating variables in cognitive difference of body image on depression eating, only trait anxiety (β = .87, 95% CI: 0.03–0.21, p < .001) had indirect influences on the relationship between cognitive difference of body image and depression eating in the female abdominal obesity group (see Fig. 1). There was significant direct influence of SES (β = .39, 95% CI: 0.14–0.61, p < .01), as the covariate, on depression eating.

Discussion

The prevalence of abdominal obesity among Chinese adults was 37.4% (45.9% in females) according to the China Health and Nutrition Surveys in 2009 [62]. This study reported that the prevalence of abdominal obesity (defined as abdomen circumference ≥ 85 cm for females) was 25% for female college student, which showed a trend towards a lower prevalence of abdominal obesity compared to those in other studies [63]. There may be two aspects related to this difference in prevalence. On the one hand, the values of the reference standards were different. On the other hand, age ranges are different. The increase was larger among individuals between the ages of 40 and 59 years [62], which suggests that the prevalence of young female abdominal obesity among the subpopulation of 19–23 with a higher level of education was lower.

In this study, all the participants (100%) perceived themselves to be fatter than physical truth and wanted a more slender figure. Indeed, wanting a thinner image was a general trend. The differences and the AC-related differences in relation of inhibitory control, emotional eating, anxiety and body image between abdominal obesity and non-abdominal obesity in young women were compared. Abdominal obesity female youths and non-abdominal obesity ones showed statistically significant differences in body image (cognitive difference and expectational difference), anxiety (trait anxiety and state anxiety), which has already been seen in previous studies [36, 40]. In all SST measure variables, no statistically significant differences were found between two groups. These results are consistent with previous researches about female undergraduates motivated to manage weight and they found no correlation between SSRT in food trials, other any SST measure and BMI [64,65,66]. Contrary to our hypothesis, non-abdominal obesity female obtained higher scores in emotional eating variables, especially anxiety eating and positive emotion eating. One reason may be that self-reported emotional eating is not related to greater food intake [67, 68]. People may be considerably biased in their ability to identify themselves as emotional eaters [69,70,71], whether they are fat or not. These results lead us to think that self-reported emotional eating measure may actually reflect eating concerns [70].

Interestingly, cognitive difference of body image have been associated with the SST, including SSACC Low Calorie, SSRT Low Calorie, SSACC High Calorie, and SSRT High Calorie in all participants and non-abdominal obesity group. However, in the abdominal obesity group, the expectational difference of body image also positively associated with SSACC Low Calorie and SSACC High Calorie. This means that food- specific inhibitory control variables, especially SSACC High Calorie, only tends to be affected by expectational body image in abdominal obesity female groups. One possible explanation is that the abdominal obesity individuals desire to be thinner, but all the females often considered themself fatter than current body shape. One’s appearance is becoming increasingly important in modern society [72]. Hence, more normal body shape girls perceived themselves to be fatter than current body shape [73].

Results revealed that cognitive difference of body image is positively associated with emotional eating for female abdominal obesity individuals, but not for their non-abdominal obesity counterparts, in which cognitive difference of body image is negatively associated with emotional eating. Given prior findings linking body image dissatisfaction to obesity [74] and emotion to obesity risk [75], this study suggests the need for additional research on the potential mediating mechanisms linking these factors. In other words, one potential explanation for the discrepancy between the effects of cognitive difference of body image on emotional eating relates to whether these relationships are mediated by some other emotion. Emotional eating is more commonly reported by women than men [76], and it is often triggered by anxiety [77]. A potential mediating mechanism is anxiety given that social physique anxiety has been observed in individuals with body image dissatisfaction to obesity [78].

Our finding that emotional eating was linked to trait and state anxiety may reflect the fact that anxiety involves the avoidance motivational system and is subject to regulation by strategies such as eating [35]. These results are in keeping with those obtained in studies by other author [40]. The results of this study show that obese individuals high in negative affect may consume more calories in response to a negative mood compared to obese individuals low in negative affect, and to lean individuals. An important finding of this study was that trait and state anxiety mediated the relationship between cognitive difference of body image and depression eating in female abdominal obesity individuals. Our studies could help elucidate the role of cognitive difference of body image in vulnerability to emotional eating among female abdominal obesity individuals with high anxiety.

This study has several limitations worth noting. First, the study was limited by its cross-sectional design and cause-effect relationships could not be established. Therefore, future studies using a longitudinal design will be required to clarify the direction of these associations. Second, emotional eating was collected from female youths using self-report questionnaires. It remains a possibility that people are unable to accurately report on their own emotional eating behaviour [79]. An alternative to self-report questionnaires as the criterion for emotional eating is to use a highly valid experimental food paradigm. Third, a convenient sample of students was used in the present study due to the complexity and particularity of the Food- inhibitory control testing process and the constraints of both financial and human resources. This sample was comprised purely of students from Xi’an of Shaanxi province, likely representing a collectivistic culture.

Conclusion

Results demonstrated that anxiety mediated the relationship between cognitive difference of body image and depressed eating in young females with abdominal obesity. In addition, only among abdominal obesity individuals, expectational difference of body image were significantly and positively correlated with food-specific inhibitory control and trait/state anxiety. The findings suggest it is of critical importance to promote a healthy body image recognition and expectation and improve mood regulation for young females with abdominal obesity high in trait anxiety.

Availability of data and materials

The data used during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

BMI:

Body Mass Index

SMT:

Silhouette-Matching Test

STAI:

State-Trait Anxiety Inventory

SST:

Stop Signal Task

EES:

Emotional Eating Scale

AC:

Abdomen Circumference

EDs:

Eating Disorders

SSRT:

Stop Signal Reaction Time

References

  1. Sharma AM. Adipose tissue: a mediator of cardiovascular risk. Int J Obes Relat Metab Disord. 2002;26(supplement 4):S5 https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0802210.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Bigaard J, Frederiksen K, TjoNneland A, Thomsen BL, Overvad K, Heitmann BL, et al. Waist circumference and body composition in relation to all-cause mortality in middle-aged men and women. Int J Obes. 2005;29(7):778–84 https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0802976.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  3. Calderón C, Forns M, Varea V. Obesidad infantil: ansiedad y síntomas cognitivos y conductuales propios de los trastornos de alimentación. Anales de Pediatría. 2009;71(6):489–94 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anpedi.2009.07.030.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Cornier MA, Tate CW, Grunwald GK, Bessesen DH. Relationship between waist circumference, body mass index, and medical care costs. Obes Res. 2002;10(11):1167–72 https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2002.158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Rimm EB, Hu FB, Stampfer MJ, Willett WC, Wang Y. Comparison of abdominal adiposity and overall obesity in predicting risk of type 2 diabetes among men. Am J Clin Nutr. 2005;81(3):555–63 https://doi.org/10.1556/AAlim.34.2005.1.12.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Kumanyika SK, Obarzanek E, Stettler N, Bell R, Field AE, Fortmann SP, et al. Population-based prevention of obesity. Circulation. 2008;118(4):428–64 https://doi.org/10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.108.189702.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Pi-Sunyer FX. The epidemiology of central fat distribution in relation to disease. Nutr Rev. 2004; https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1753-4887.2004.tb00081.x.

  8. Xi B. Secular trends in the prevalence of general and abdominal obesity among Chinese adults, 1993–2009. Obes Rev. 2012;13(3):287–96 https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-789x.2011.00944.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Kang SM, Yoon JW, Ahn HY, Kim SY, Lee KH, Shin H, et al. Android fat depot is more closely associated with metabolic syndrome than abdominal visceral fat in elderly people. PLoS One. 2011;6:e27694 https://10.1371/journal.pone.0027694.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  10. Sucharda P. Abdominal obesity. Cas Lek Cesk. 2009;148(2):78–82.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Moreira RO, Marca KF, Appolinario JC, Coutinho WF. Increased waist circumference is associated with an increased prevalence of mood disorders and depressive symptoms in obese women. Eat Weight Disord. 2007;12(1):35–40 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03327770.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Heo M, Pietrobelli A, Fontaine KR, Sirey JA, Faith MS. Depressive mood and obesity in us adults: comparison and moderation by sex, age, and race. Int J Obes. 2006;30(3):513–9 https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.ijo.0803122.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  13. Kang MH, Choue R. Relationships of body image, body stress and eating attitude, and dietary quality in middle school girls based on their BMI. Korean J Nutr. 2010;43(3) https://doi.org/10.4163/kjn.2010.43.3.285.

  14. Strine TW, Mokdad AH, Dube SR, Balluz LS, Gonzalez O, Berry JT, et al. The association of depression and anxiety with obesity and unhealthy behaviors among community-dwelling us adults. Gen Hosp Psychiatry. 2008;30(2):127–37 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2007.12.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Sutter C, Ontai L, Scherr R, Linnell J, Nicholson Y, Spezzano T, et al. Individual differences in associations between parenting practices and BMI: the role of inhibitory control. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2014;46(4):S136 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneb.2014.04.104.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Zhao G, Ford ES, Dhingra S, Li C, Strine TW, Mokdad AH. Depression and anxiety among us adults: associations with body mass index. Int J Obes, (2005). 2009;33(2):257–66 https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2008.268.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  17. Anderson SE, Cohen P, Naumova EN, Must A. Association of depression and anxiety disorders with weight change in a prospective community-based study of children followed up into adulthood. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2006;160(3):285 https://doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.160.3.285.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Solomon MR. Eating as both coping and stressor in overweight control. J Adv Nurs. 2001;36(4):563–72 https://doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2648.2001.02009.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  19. Geliebter A, Aversa A. Emotional eating in overweight, normal weight, and underweight individuals. Eat Behav. 2003;3(4):341–7 https://doi.org/10.1016/S1471-0153(02)00100-9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Flynn KJ, Fitzgibbon M. Body images and obesity risk among black females: a review of the literature. Ann Behav Med. 1998;20(1):13–24 https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02893804.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Gilbert-Diamond D, Baylin A, Mora-Plazas M, Villamor E. Correlates of obesity and body image in colombian women. J Women's Health. 2009;18(8):1145 https://doi.org/10.1089/jwh.2008.1179.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  22. So ES. Perceptual body image and the relationship with weight control across the adult lifespan by sex in Koreans. J Public Health. 2017;39(4):777–86 https://doi.org/10.1093/pubmed/fdx021.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  23. Liang J, Matheson B, Kaye W, Boutelle K. Neurocognitive correlates of obesity and obesity-related behaviors in children and adolescents. Int J Obes. 2014;38:494–506 https://doi.org/10.1038/ijo.2013.142.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  24. Reinert KRS, Pe EK, Barkin SL. The relationship between executive function and obesity in children and adolescents: a systematic literature review. J Obes. 2013;82095:6 https://doi.org/10.1155/2013/820956.

    Google Scholar 

  25. Bartholdy S, Dalton B, O’Daly OG, Campbell IC, Schmidt U. A systematic review of the relationship between eating, weight and inhibitory control using the stop signal task. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016;64:35–62 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.02.010.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Chamberlain SR, Derbyshire KL, Leppink E, Grant JE. Obesity and dissociable forms of impulsivity in young adults. CNS Spectr. 2015;20:1–8 https://doi.org/10.1017/S1092852914000625.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Houben K, Nederkoorn C, Jansen A. Eating on impulse: the relation between overweight and food-specific inhibitory control. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2014;22:E6–8 https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.20670.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Grant J, Derbyshire K, Leppink E, Chamberlain S. Obesity and gambling: neurocognitive and clinical associations. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2015;131:379–86 https://doi.org/10.1111/acps.12353.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Hendrick OM, Luo X, Zhang S, Li CS. Saliency processing and obesity: a preliminary imaging study of the stop signal task. Obesity. 2012;20:1796–802 https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2011.180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Lawyer SR, Boomhower SR, Rasmussen EB. Differential associations between obesity and behavioral measures of impulsivity. Appetite. 2015;95:375–82 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2015.07.031.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Nederkoorn C. Effects of sales promotions, weight status, and impulsivity on purchases in a supermarket. Obesity. 2014;22:E2–5 https://doi.org/10.1002/oby.20621.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Greeno CG, Wing RR. Stress-induced eating. Psychol Bull. 1994;115(3):444–64 https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.444.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. Allison DB, Heska S. Emotion and eating in obesity? A critical analysis. Int J Eating Disord. 1993;13(3):289–95 https://doi.org/10.1002/1098-108x(199304)13:3<289::aid-eat2260130307>3.0.co;2-x.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  34. Teixeira MD, Pereira AT, Marques MV, Saraiva JM, de Macedo António F. Eating behaviors, body image, perfectionism, and self-esteem in a sample of Portuguese girls. Rev Bras Psiquiatr. 2016;38(2):135–40 https://doi.org/10.1590/1516-4446-2015-1723.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  35. Schneider KL, Appelhans BM, Whited MC, Oleski J, Pagoto SL. Trait anxiety, but not trait anger, predisposes obese individuals to emotional eating. Appetite, (APPET). 2010;55(3):701–6 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2010.10.006.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Cooper M, Burrows A. Behav Cognit Psychother. 2001;29(2):143–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Stoeber J, Yang H. Physical appearance perfectionism explains variance in eating disorder symptoms above general perfectionism. Personal Individ Differ. 2015;86:303–7 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.032.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Atlantis E, Goldney RD, Wittert GA. Obesity and depression or anxiety. BMJ. 2009;339 https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.b3868.

  39. Vingilis ER, Wade TJ, Seeley JS. Predictors of adolescent self-rated health. Analysis of the national population health survey. Can J Public Health. 2002;93(3):193–7 https://doi.org/10.1080/03014460110079455.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  40. Jansen A, Vanreyten A, van Balveren T, Roefs A, Nederkoorn C, Havermans R. Negative affect and cue-induced overeating in non-eating disordered obesity. Appetite. 2008;51(3):556–62 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2008.04.009.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Braet C, Beyers W, Goossens L, Verbeken S, Moens E. Subtyping children and adolescents who are overweight based on eating pathology and psychopathology. Eur Eat Disord Rev. 2012;20(4):279–86 https://doi.org/10.1002/erv.1151.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Petry NM, Barry D, Pietrzak RH, Wagner JA. Overweight and obesity are associated with psychiatric disorders: results from the national epidemiologic survey on alcohol and related conditions. Psychosom Med. 2008;70(3):288–97 https://doi.org/10.1097/psy.0b013e3181651651.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  43. Ball K, Burton NW, Brown WJ. A prospective study of overweight, physical activity, and depressive symptoms in young women. Obesity. 2008;17(1):66–71 https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2008.497.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Fu Q, Land KC. The increasing prevalence of overweight and obesity of children and youth in China, 1989–2009: an age–period–cohort analysis. Popul Res Policy Rev. 2015;34(6):901–21 https://doi.org/10.1007/s11113-015-9372-y.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Hu L, Huang X, You C, Li J, Hong K, Li P, et al. Prevalence of overweight, obesity, abdominal obesity and obesity-related risk factors in southern China. PLoS One. 2017;12(9):e0183934 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183934.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  46. Reynolds K, Gu D, Whelton PK, Wu X, Duan X, Mo J, et al. Prevalence and risk factors of overweight and obesity in China. Obesity. 2007;15(1):10–8 https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2007.527.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Li C, Ford ES, Mcguire LC, Mokdad AH. Increasing trends in waist circumference and abdominal obesity among u.s. adults. Obesity. 2007;15(1):216 https://doi.org/10.1038/oby.2007.505.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  48. Wimmelmann CL, Lund R, Flensborg-Madsen T, Christensen U, Osler M, Mortensen EL. Associations of personality with body mass index and obesity in a large late midlife community sample. Obes Facts. 2018;11(2):129–43 https://doi.org/10.1159/000487888.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  49. Wang L, Sun N. Ps 11-48 impact of taurine supplementation on abdominal obesity high-normal blood pressure. J Hypertens. 2016;34:e347 https://doi.org/10.1097/01.hjh.0000500880.58252.70.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Peterson M, Ellenberg D, Crossan S. Body-image perceptions: reliability of a BMI-based Silhouette matching test. Am J Health Behav. 2003;27:355–63 https://doi.org/10.5993/AJHB.27.4.7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  51. Thompson JK, Heinberg LJ, Altabe MN, TantleffDunn S. Exacting beauty: theory, assessment, and treatment of body image disturbance. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association; 1999.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  52. Julian LJ. Measures of anxiety: state-trait anxiety inventory (STAI), beck anxiety inventory (BAI), and hospital anxiety and depression scale-anxiety (HADS-A). Arthritis Care Res. 2011;63(supplement S11):S467–72 https://doi.org/10.1002/acr.20561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  53. Shek DTL. Reliability and factorial structure of the Chinese version of the state-trait anxiety inventory. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. 1988;10(4):303–17 https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00960624.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Svaldi J, Naumann E, Trentowska M, Schmitz F. General and food-specific inhibitory deficits in binge eating disorder. Int J Eat Disord. 2014;47(5):534–42.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: A model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1984;10(2):276–91.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  56. Arnow B, Kenardy J, Agras WS. The emotional eating scale: the development of a measure to assess coping with negative affect by eating. Int J Eat Disord. 1995;18:79–90 http://dx.doi.org/0.1002/1098-108x(199507)18:1<79::aid-eat2260180109>3.0.co;2-v.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Zhu H, Cai T, Chen G, Zhang B. Validation of the emotional eating scale among Chinese undergraduates. Soc Behav Personal Int J. 2013;41(1):123–34 https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2013.41.1.123.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Bornstein MH, Bradley RH. Socioeconomic status, parenting, and child development. Mahwah: Erlbaum; 2003.

    Google Scholar 

  59. Hollingshead AB. Four factor index of social status. New Haven: Yale University Press; 1975.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Arbuckle JL. Amos (version 7.0) [computer program]. Chicago: SPSS; 2006.

    Google Scholar 

  61. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: conventional criteria versus. Structural equation modeling. Struct Equ Model Multidiscip J. 1999;6(1):1–55.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Jansen HJ, van Essen P, Koenen T, Joosten LAB, Netea MG, Tack CJ, Stienstra R. Autophagy Activity Is Up-Regulated in Adipose Tissue of Obese Individuals and Modulates Proinflammatory Cytokine Expression. Endocrinology. 2012;153(12):5866–74.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  63. Wang H, Wang J, Liu MM, Wang D, Liu YQ, Zhao Y, et al. Epidemiology of general obesity, abdominal obesity and related risk factors in urban adults from 33 communities of Northeast China: the CHPSNE study. BMC Public Health. 2012;12(1) https://doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-12-967.

  64. Haynes A, Kemps E, Moffitt R. The moderating role of state inhibitory control in the effect of evaluative conditioning on temptation and unhealthy snacking. Physiol Behav. 2015;152:135–42 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.09.020.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Lokken KL, Boeka AG, Austin HM, Gunstad J, Harmon CM. Evidence of executive dysfunction in extremely obese adolescents: a pilot study. Surg Obes Relat Dis. 2009;5:547–52 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.soard.2009.05.008.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Meule A, Lutz AP, Vogele C, Kubler A. Impulsive reactions to food-cues predict subsequent food craving. Eat Behav. 2014;15:99–105 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.eatbeh.2013.10.023.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Braden A, Flatt SW, Boutelle KN, Strong D, Sherwood NE, Rock CL. Emotional eating is associated with weight loss success among adults enrolled in a weight loss program. J Behav Med. 2016;39(4):727–32 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10865-016-9728-8.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  68. Braden A, Rhee K, Peterson CB, Rydell SA, Zucker N, Boutelle K. Associations between child emotional eating and general parenting style, feeding practices, and parent psychopathology. Appetite. 2014;80:35–40.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Adriaanse MA, Prinsen S, Huberts d W, Jessie C, de Ridder DT, Evers C. ‘I ate too much so I must have been sad’: emotions as a confabulated reason for overeating. Appetite. 2016;103:318–23 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2016.04.028.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Evers C, De Ridder DTD, Adriaanse MA. Assessing yourself as an emotional eater: Mission impossible? Appetite. 2011;57(2):536.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Evers C, de Ridder DTD, Adriaanse MA. Adequately predicting emotional eating with self-reports: Not as easy as pie. Health Psychol. 2010;29(4):344–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  72. Watts J. China’s cosmetic surgery craze. Lancet. 2004;363:958 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140-6736(04)15832-7.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Bašková M, Holubčíková J, Baška T. Body-image dissatisfaction and weight-control behaviour in slovak adolescents. Cent Eur J Public Health. 2017;25(3):216–21 https://doi.org/10.21101/cejph.a4724.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  74. Kostanski M, Gullone E. Adolescent body image dissatisfaction: relationships with self-esteem, anxiety, and depression controlling for body mass. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1998;39(2):255–62 https://doi.org/10.1111/1469-7610.00319.

    CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  75. Fernandes J, Ferreira-Santos F, Miller K, Torres S. Emotional processing in obesity: a systematic review and exploratory meta-analysis. Obes Rev. 2018;19(1):111–20 https://doi.org/10.1111/obr.12607.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Beydoun MA. The interplay of gender, mood, and stress hormones in the association between emotional eating and dietary behavior. J Nutr. 2014;144(8):1139–41 https://doi.org/10.3945/jn.114.196717.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  77. Walfish S, Brown TA. Self-assessed emotional factors contributing to increased weight in presurgical male bariatric patients. Bariatric Nurs Surg Patient Care. 2009;4(1):49–52 https://doi.org/10.1089/bar.2009.9991.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Tok S, Catikkas F, Canpolat AM, Koyuncu M. Body image satisfaction and dissatisfaction, social physique anxiety, self-esteem, and body fat ratio in female exercisers and nonexercisers. Soc Behav Pers An Int J. 2010;38(4):561–70 https://doi.org/10.2224/sbp.2010.38.4.561.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Braden A, Emley E, Watford T, Anderson LN, Musher-Eizenman D. Self-reported emotional eating is not related to greater food intake: results from two laboratory studies. Psychol Health. 2019;28 https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2019.1649406.

Download references

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the study participants for their time and effort.

Funding

This work was supported by grants from Social Science Foundation of Shaanxi Province (2020 M007), China Postdoctoral Science Foundation (2020 M683444), and the Fundamental Research Funds for Xi’an Jiaotong University (SK2020019).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

All authors made significant contributions to the paper. ZHH and MDL identified the research question and designed the study. Data collection and analysis were carried out by (ZHH) and then discussed and revised with all authors (ZHH, MDL, CJL, and XYM). All authors (ZHH, MDL, CJL, and XYM) read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Zhong-Hua He.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All procedures performed in studies involving human participants were in accordance with the ethical standards of Biomedical Ethics Committee of the Medical Department of Xi’an Jiaotong University. Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Consent for publication

Written informed consent for publication was obtained from all participants.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no conflict of interest.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

He, ZH., Li, MD., Liu, CJ. et al. Relationship between body image, anxiety, food-specific inhibitory control, and emotional eating in young women with abdominal obesity: a comparative cross-sectional study. Arch Public Health 79, 11 (2021). https://doi.org/10.1186/s13690-021-00526-2

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s13690-021-00526-2

Keywords